Apple Is Happy to Use Women and People of Color as Art Not Authority

Study of influence of colour on man behavior

The "rose of temperaments" (Temperamenten-Rose) compiled by Goethe and Schiller in 1798/9. The diagram matches twelve colors to human being occupations or their grapheme traits, grouped in the four temperaments: * choleric (red/orangish/yellow): tyrants, heroes, adventurers * sanguine (yellow/greenish/cyan) hedonists, lovers, poets * phlegmatic (cyan/blue/violet): public speakers, historians * melancholic (violet/magenta/red): philosophers, pedants, rulers

Color psychology is the study of hues as a determinant of human being behavior. Carl Jung has been credited equally one of the pioneers in this field for his explorations into the properties and meanings of colors in our lives.[ commendation needed ] Color influences perceptions that are not obvious, such as the taste of food. Colors have qualities that can cause certain emotions in people.[1] Colors can also enhance the effectiveness of placebos.[2] For instance, red or orange pills are generally used equally stimulants.[2] How color influences individuals may differ depending on age, gender, and civilisation. For example, heterosexual men tend to report that blood-red outfits enhance female bewitchery, while heterosexual females deny whatever outfit color impacting that of men.[3] Although color associations tin can vary contextually between cultures, color preference is to be relatively uniform beyond gender and race.[4]

Colour psychology is also widely used in marketing and branding. Marketers see color as important, equally colour tin can influence a consumers' emotions and perceptions about goods and services. Logos for companies are important, since the logos can attract more customers. This happens when customers believe the visitor logo matches the personality of the goods and services, such as the colour pinkish heavily used on Victoria's Secret branding.[5] Colors are also important for window displays in stores. Research shows that colors such every bit red tended to concenter spontaneous purchasers, despite cool colors such every bit blue being more favorable.[half-dozen] Red and yellow, equally a combination, can stimulate hunger, which may aid to explicate, in part, the success of fast-nutrient restaurants such as McDonald'due south, Burger King, and In-Northward-Out Burger.[7] The phenomenon has been referred to as the "ketchup & mustard" theory.[8]

While marketing makes lucrative use of color psychology'due south principles, the applications of the field bear on many other domains such as in medical therapies, sports, hospital settings, and even in game design.

History [edit]

Before there was color psychology equally a field, color was being used for centuries as a method of treatment as early every bit 2000 BC. The ancient Egyptians documented color "cures" using painted rooms or sunlight shining through crystals every bit therapy. 1 of the primeval medical documents, the Nei Ching, documents color diagnoses associated with color healing practices.[9]

Carl Jung is well-nigh prominently associated with the pioneering stages of color psychology in the 20th century. Jung was most interested in colors' properties and meanings, besides every bit in fine art's potential as a tool for psychotherapy. His studies in and writings on color symbolism embrace a broad range of topics, from mandalas to the works of Picasso to the near-universal sovereignty of the color gold, the lattermost of which, co-ordinate to Charles A. Riley II, "expresses... the apex of spirituality, and intuition".[ten] In pursuing his studies of color usage and effects across cultures and time periods, as well equally in examining his patients' self-created mandalas, Jung attempted to unlock and develop a language, or code, the ciphers of which would be colors. He looked to alchemy to farther his understanding of the surreptitious language of color, finding the cardinal to his research in alchemical transmutation. His work has historically informed the modern field of color psychology.

Model of colour psychology [edit]

The general model of color psychology relies on six basic principles:

  1. Colour tin can bear a specific pregnant.
  2. Color significant is either based in learned meaning or biologically innate meaning.
  3. The perception of a color causes evaluation automatically by the person perceiving.
  4. The evaluation procedure forces color-motivated behavior.
  5. Color unremarkably exerts its influence automatically.
  6. Color significant and effect has to do with context as well.[6]

Influence of color on perception [edit]

Multiple researchers propose that 1 factor in the evolution of primate trichromatic color vision is to allow for better perception of others' emotions or status which can show highly useful for complex social interaction.[eleven] For instance, flushed or stake skin can not-verbally communicate whether they are excited or sickly. Besides its use for social situations, colour has an touch on in multiple facets of our perceptions.

Taste [edit]

Color also affects how people perceive the edibility and flavour of foods and drinks.[12] Non only the color of the food itself but likewise that of everything in the eater'due south field of vision tin affect this. For example, in food stores, bread is normally sold in packaging decorated or tinted with golden or brown tones to promote the thought of home baked and oven freshness.[13] People can mistake a crimson flavored beverage for being lime or lemon flavored if that drinkable was a green colour. Additionally, a flavour can exist intensified by a colour. People tin charge per unit a brown M&M as more chocolate flavored than a green One thousand&K based on color.[12] This interaction can be mediated past our perceptions as well, specially depending on cultural expectation. Research in the UK demonstrated that individuals receiving a brownish drink would have different expectations for the taste (i.e. expecting a Cola) while someone from Taiwan may await a grape flavored drink because of popular drinks in their culture.[14]

Time [edit]

Recent results[15] showed that the perceived duration of a red screen was longer than was that of a blue screen. The results reflected sex differences; men, simply not women, overestimated the duration of the red screen. Additionally, the reaction times to a red screen were faster than those to a blue screen. Participants who reacted quickly to a red screen overestimated its duration. In a demo with 150 people chosen at random, it was found that inside a pod bathed in bluish color the average perceived duration of a minute was xi seconds shorter than in a pod bathed in red color.[xvi] However, another study looking at perceived duration institute reverse results regarding bluish and ruby-red stimuli.[17]

Light [edit]

Light and colour influence how people encounter their surroundings. Different types of calorie-free sources impact interior or external objects by visually changing their surface colors. Specific hues observed under natural sunlight may vary when seen under the light from an incandescent (tungsten) light-bulb: lighter colors may appear to be more orange or "brownish" and darker colors may appear fifty-fifty darker.[18] Calorie-free and the color of an object tin can affect how 1 perceives its positioning. If light or shadow, or the color of the object, masks an object'due south true contour (outline of a effigy) information technology can appear to be shaped differently from reality.[18] Objects nether a uniform light-source will promote ameliorate impression of 3-dimensional shape.[xviii] The color of an object may impact whether or non it seems to be in motion. In particular, the trajectories of objects under a calorie-free source whose intensity varies with space are more difficult to decide than identical objects under a compatible light source. This could possibly be interpreted as interference between motion and color perception, both of which are more difficult under variable lighting.[xviii]

Bluish light causes people to feel relaxed, which has led countries to add blue street lights in order to decrease suicide rates.[xix] In 2000, the city of Glasgow installed blue street lighting in certain neighborhoods and subsequently reported the anecdotal finding of reduced criminal offense in these areas.[20] [21] A railroad company in Japan installed blue lighting on its stations in October 2009 in an effort to reduce the number of suicide attempts,[22] although the effect of this technique has been questioned.[23]

Medicine [edit]

The color of placebo pills is reported to be a factor in their effectiveness, with "hot-colored" (carmine, yellowish, etc.) pills working meliorate as stimulants and "cool-colored" (blue, majestic, etc.) pills working better as depressants. This relationship is believed to exist a consequence of the patient's expectations and not a direct effect of the color itself.[2] Consequently, these effects appear to be culture-dependent.[24] l

Color preference and the connection betwixt color and emotion [edit]

How people respond to different color stimuli varies from person to person. In a U.S. study, blue is the top choice at 35%, followed past green (16%), purple (ten%) and ruby (9%).[25] Blueish and green may be due to a preference for sure habitats that were beneficial in the ancestral environment equally explained in evolutionary aesthetics.[26] Orange, xanthous, and brown are the least popular colors, respectively.[27]

One theory for why people prefer one color over another is chosen ecological valence theory (EVT) proposed by Stephen Palmer and Karen Schloss.[28] This theory asserts that people tend to like or dislike colors based on their associations of the colour to other objects or situations that they have strong feelings nigh. For example, if someone associates the color bluish with make clean h2o, they would be more likely to favor blue. On the other hand, people'due south dislike of the color chocolate-brown could be due to associations of it with feces or rotten food.

Color preference may as well depend on ambient temperature. People who are cold ofttimes select warm colors such every bit red or yellow, while people who are hot favor cool colors like blueish and greenish.[6] Introverted individuals are also plant to be more attracted to cool colors, while extroverts prefer warmer colors.[29]

Psychologist Andrew J. Elliot tested to see if the colour of a person'due south vesture could make them appear more than sexually appealing. He found heterosexual men and women dressed in red were significantly more likely to attract romantic attention than women dressed in any other color. The colour did non affect heterosexual women'south cess of other women's bewitchery. Other studies have shown men dressed in red appeal to heterosexual women.[3]

Uses in marketing [edit]

Since colour is an of import gene in the visual appearance of products also equally in make recognition, color psychology has become important to marketing. Recent work in marketing has shown that color can exist used to communicate brand personality.[30]

Marketers must be enlightened of the application of color in different media (east.thou. print vs. web), likewise as the varying meanings and emotions that a particular audition can assign to colour. Fifty-fifty though there are attempts to classify consumer response to unlike colors, everyone perceives color differently. The physiological and emotional result of color in each person is influenced by several factors such as past experiences, civilization, religion, natural surround, gender, race, and nationality. When making color decisions, information technology is important to determine the target audition in guild to convey the right message. Color decisions can influence both directly messages and secondary make values and attributes in any advice. Color should exist carefully selected to align with the fundamental message and emotions being conveyed in a piece.[31]

Research on the effects of color on product preference and marketing shows that product color could bear on consumer preference and hence purchasing culture. This is mostly due to associative learning. Well-nigh results bear witness that it is not a specific colour that attracts all audiences, but that certain colors are accounted appropriate for certain products.[32]

Make meaning [edit]

Color is a very influential source of information when people are making a purchasing conclusion.[33] Customers by and large brand an initial judgment on a production within 90 seconds of interaction with that product and most 62%-xc% of that judgment is based on color.[33] People often see the logo of a brand or company as a representation of that company. Without prior experience to a logo, we begin to acquaintance a make with certain characteristics based on the primary logo color.[34]

Color mapping provides a ways of identifying potential logo colors for new brands and ensuring brand differentiation inside a visually cluttered marketplace.[35] [ failed verification ]

A written report on logo color asked participants to charge per unit how advisable the logo color was for fictional companies based on the products each company produced. Participants were presented with fictional products in eight different colors and had to rate the appropriateness of the color for each product. This study showed a blueprint of logo colour appropriateness based on product part. If the production was considered functional, fulfills a need or solves a problem, and so a functional color was seen as most appropriate. If the product was seen every bit sensory-social, conveys attitudes, status, or social blessing, so sensory-social colors were seen as more advisable.[34] Companies should decide what types of products to produce then cull a logo color that is connotative with their products' functions.

Company logos can portray meaning just through the apply of colour.[36] Color affects people'south perceptions of a new or unknown company. Some companies such as Victoria's Secret and H&R Block used color to change their corporate image and create a new brand personality for a specific target audience.[36] Research done on the relationship between logo color and five personality traits had participants charge per unit a figurer-made logo in unlike colors on scales relating to the dimensions of brand personality. Relationships were constitute betwixt color and sincerity, excitement, competence, sophistication, and ruggedness. A follow upwards study tested the effects of perceived make personality and purchasing intentions.[36] Participants were presented with a product and a summary of the preferred brand personality and had to rate the likelihood of purchasing a product based on packaging color. Purchasing intent was greater if the perceived personality matched the marketed product or service. In turn color affects perceived brand personality and brand personality affects purchasing intent.[36]

Although color tin be useful in marketing, its value and extent of use depends on how it is used and the audience information technology is used on.[37] The use of colour will have different effects on dissimilar people, therefore experimental findings cannot be taken as universally true.

Specific color meaning [edit]

Different colors are perceived to hateful dissimilar things. For case, tones of reddish lead to feelings of arousal while blue tones are oftentimes associated with feelings of relaxation. Both of these emotions are pleasant, so therefore, the colors themselves can procure positive feelings in advertisements. The chart below gives perceived meanings of different colors in the U.s..

Functional (F): fulfills a need or solves a problem[34]

Sensory-Social (S): conveys attitudes, status, or social approval[34]

Scarlet Xanthous Green Blue Pink Violet/Purple Orange Brown Black White
Lust (Due south)[38] Competence (S)[36] Proficient Taste (F)[38] Masculine (S)[38] Sophistication (Southward)[36] Authority (South)[38] Warmth (S)[39] Ruggedness (S)[36] Grief (Due south)[38] Happiness (S)[38]
Ability (S)[40] Happiness (South)[38] Envy (Due south)[38] Competence (S)[36] Sincerity (S)[36] Sophistication (S)[36] Excitement (Southward)[39] Sophistication (S)[36] Sincerity (S)[36]
Excitement (S)[36] Inexpensive (F)[27] Eco-Friendly (F) [41] High quality (F)[38] Feminine and Flirty (S)[38] Ability (S)[38] Expensive (F)[38] Purity (Southward)[38]
Love (Due south)[38] Low Quality (F)[27] Wellness (Due south) [41] Corporate (F)[38] Fear (S)[38]
Speed (Southward) [four] Money (F)[42] Reliability (F) [27]
Anger (S)[43]

Meanings in cartography [edit]

This map of atmospheric precipitation leverages the natural connotations of blueish as wet and yellow as dry, but readers have to make a conscious try to not translate light-green as vegetation

In map design, boosted color meanings are commonly employed to create intuitive map symbols, due to the natural colors of common geographic features.[44] These correlations are commonly stylized and conventionalized, then that the color with the most intuitive meaning is often the nearest prototypical named colour rather than that nearly like to the real-world color (e.g., in very rare locations is water as deep and pure a blue as is commonly used in maps). Common (simply by no means authoritative or exhaustive) examples include:

  • Greenish: vegetation
  • Blue: water (water bodies, precipitation), cold
  • Yellow: dryness
  • Brownish: soil
  • Red: rut, wildfire
  • Purple: unnatural (contrasting with natural connotations of green, yellow, blue)
  • Gray/Black: human structures (roads, buildings)

Other colors tin have intuitive pregnant due to their role in Gestalt psychology and other cognitive aspects of the map-reading process. For instance, shades that contrast most with the background (i.east., dark on a white page, lite on a nighttime screen) are naturally perceived as "more" (college values of quantitative properties, more important in the Visual hierarchy) than shades with less contrast.

Combining colors [edit]

Although some companies use a single colour to represent their brand, many other companies utilize a combination of colors in their logo, and can be perceived in different ways than those colors independently. When asked to rate color pair preference of preselected pairs, people generally adopt color pairs with similar hues when the two colors are both in the foreground; however, greater contrast between the effigy and the background is preferred.[45]

In contrast to a stiff preference for similar colour combinations, some people like to emphasis with a highly contrasting color.[46] In a study on colour preference for Nike, Inc. sneakers, people mostly combined colors near each other on the color bike, such every bit blue and dark blue. Still, a smaller segment preferred to accept the Nike swoosh accentuated in a different, and contrasting, colour. About of the people also used a relatively small number of colors when designing their platonic athletic shoe. This finding has relevance for companies that produce multicolored merchandise, suggesting that to appeal to consumer preferences, companies should consider minimizing the number of colors visible and using similar hues in any ane production.[47]

Colour name [edit]

Although different colors can be perceived in unlike means, the names of those colors matters as well.[47] [48] These names are oft called visual color descriptors. Many products and companies focus on producing a wide range of product colors to concenter the largest population of consumers. For example, cosmetics brands produce a rainbow of colors for heart shadow and nail polish, to appeal to every blazon of person. Even companies such as Apple Inc. and Dell who make iPods and laptops exercise so with a sure amount of color personalization available to attract buyers. Moreover, color name, non merely the actual color, can attract or repel buyers likewise. When asked to rate color swatches and products with either generic color names (such as brown) or "fancy" colour names (such every bit mocha), participants rated items with fancy names as significantly more likable than items with generic names.[47] In fact, the same paint color swatch with ii different names produced different rating levels, and the same effect was found when participants rated the pleasantness of towels given fancy or generic color names,[47] showing an overall pattern of preference for fancy color names over generic ones when describing exactly the same colour.

Furthermore, it would appear that in addition to fancy names being preferred for their aural entreatment, they may actually contribute to the production they represent itself being liked more, and hence in this manner impact sales.[49] A yellowish jelly bean with an atypical color name such equally razzmatazz is more likely to be selected than ane with a more typical proper noun such as lemon yellow. This could exist due to greater interest in atypical names, likewise as marvel and willingness to "figure out" why that name was chosen. Purchasing intent patterns regarding custom sweatshirts from an online vendor likewise revealed a preference for atypical names. Participants were asked to imagine ownership sweatshirts and were provided with a variety of color name options, some typical, some atypical. Color names that were atypical were selected more often than typical color names, over again confirming a preference for atypical color names and for item descriptions using those names.[49] Moreover, those who chose sweatshirts bearing atypical color names were described as more content with their purchase than those who selected like items begetting typical color names.

Attracting attention [edit]

A store sign painted mainly cherry-red on a street in Bangkok to attract attention from passers-by

Colour is used every bit a means to attract consumer attending to a production that then influences buying behavior.[50] Consumers use colour to identify for known brands or search for new alternatives. Diverseness seekers expect for non-typical colors when selecting new brands. Bonny color packaging receives more consumer attention than unattractive colour packaging, which can and so influence buying behavior. A report that looked at visual colour cues focused on predicted purchasing behavior for known and unknown brands.[50] Participants were shown the same product in iv different colors and brands. The results showed that people picked packages based on colors that attracted their voluntary and involuntary attention. Associations made with that colour such every bit 'green fits menthol', also affected their decision. Based on these findings implications tin be fabricated on the best colour choices for packages. New companies or new products could consider using dissimilar colors to attract attending to the brand, simply off-brand companies could consider using similar colors to the leading brand to emphasize production similarity. If a visitor is changing the expect of a product, merely keeping the product the same, they consider keeping the aforementioned color scheme since people use colour to identify and search for brands.[50] This can be seen in Crayola crayons, where the logo has changed many times since 1934, just the basic package colors, gold and green, accept been kept throughout.

Attention is captured subconsciously before people tin can consciously attend to something.[51] Research looking at electroencephalography (EEGs) while people made decisions on color preference plant brain activation when a favorite colour is present before the participants consciously focused on it. When looking at various colors on a screen people focus on their favorite color, or the colour that stands out more, before they purposefully plough their attention to information technology. This implies that products can capture someone's attention based on color, before the person willingly looks at the product.[51]

In interactive design and behavioral design, color is used to develop visual hierarchies where colors are placed into saliency hierarchies that match other hierarchies. Examples include matching a color bureaucracy to a navigational structure hierarchy, or matching a behavioral science hierarchy to the well-nigh salient colors in a visual hierarchy, to increase the odds that important behavior change principles are noticed past a target audience and processed by them.[52]

Store and display color [edit]

Warm colored window display

Colour is not only used in products to attract attention, but also in window displays and stores.[53] When people are exposed to different colored walls and images of window displays and store interiors they tend to be fatigued to some colors and not to others. Findings showed that people were physically drawn to warm colored displays; even so, they rated cool colored displays as more favorable. This implies that warm colored store displays are more than appropriate for spontaneous and unplanned purchases, whereas cool colored displays and store entrances may exist a better fit for purchases where a lot of planning and customer deliberation occurs. This is especially relevant in shopping malls where patrons could easily walk into a store that attracts their attention without previous planning.[53]

Other enquiry has confirmed that store color, and not just the product, influences ownership beliefs.[48] When people are exposed to dissimilar shop color scenarios then surveyed on intended buying behavior, store color, among diverse other factors, seems important for purchasing intentions. Particularly bluish, a cool color, was rated every bit more favorable and produced higher purchasing intentions than orangish, a warm color. Yet, all negative effects to orange were neutralized when orangish shop color was paired with soft lighting. This shows that shop colour and lighting actually interact.[48]

Lighting color could have a stiff effect on perceived experience in stores and other state of affairs. For example, time seems to pass more slowly under red lights and time seems to pass apace under blue light.[33] Casinos take total advantage of this miracle by using color to get people to spend more time and hence more coin in their casino.[33] Yet, a presumed influence of colored calorie-free (red vs. blue) on risk beliefs could non be demonstrated.[54]

Applications for therapy [edit]

Art therapist Felicity Kodjo watches over a patient during an art therapy workshop.

Art therapy [edit]

Art therapy is a separate but related field of applied psychology. It comes from psychoanalytic theories in the 1970'southward that argued that some of our emotions and experiences cannot just be expressed in words, merely in images and colors.[55] One intersection where color psychology could be of use to art therapists is in evaluating what certain colors mean to clients when they employ them to create art pieces. Even the lack of color use can exist an of import item in art therapy, as people struggling with depression tend to utilise less color when they are painting.[55] It'due south also suggested that by focusing on color use rather than the resulting image made by a client, yous tin can avoid clients' feet over producing a "proficient" product and focus more on what the colors they used mean to them in guild to offset a dialogue about their feelings.[55]

Chromotherapy [edit]

Chromotherapy is a treatment method adapted from ancient color-based practices that uses wavelengths in the visual spectrum (colors we can meet) to treat dissimilar conditions.[56] This therapy has been researched to care for multiple physiological and psychiatric conditions, such as seasonal affective disorder (SAD), age-related cerebral decline, depression, and hypertension amidst others.[57] [58] [59] Still, many in the healthcare field believe that chromotherapy does not have a robust inquiry bankroll and accept described it equally a pseudoscience.[threescore] Other sources merits that while certain colors have been shown as beneficial to wellbeing, the clear definition of what wavelengths are benign and exactly how these benefits occur is not clear plenty to put them to use in a medical setting.[61]

Individual differences [edit]

Pink girls section of toy store

Gender [edit]

Children'southward toys are oftentimes categorized as either boys or girls toys solely based on colour. In a study on color furnishings on perception, adult participants were shown blurred images of children'due south toys where the merely decipherable characteristic visible was the toy's colour.[62] In general participants categorized the toys into girl and boy toys based on the visible color of the prototype. This can be seen in companies interested in marketing masculine toys, such as edifice sets, to boys. For example, Lego uses pink to specifically advertise some sets to girls rather than boys. The classification of 'daughter' and 'boy' toys on the Disney Store website besides uses color associations for each gender.[63] An analysis of the colors used showed that bold colored toys, such as reddish and blackness, were generally classified as 'boy only' toys and pastel colored toys, such as pink and majestic, were classified as 'daughter only' toys. Toys that were classified as both boy and daughter toys took on 'boy just' toy colors. This once again emphasizes the distinction in color use for children's toys.[63]

Gender differences in colour associations can also be seen amidst adults.[64] Differences were noted for male person and female participants, where the ii genders did non agree on which color pairs they enjoyed the most when presented with a multifariousness of colors.[62] [65] Men and women also did not agree on which colors should be classified as masculine and feminine. This could imply that men and women mostly adopt different colors when purchasing items. Men and women too misperceive what colors the opposite gender views equally fitting for them.

Gender has also shown to influence how colors are received, with some research suggesting women and men respectively adopt "warm" and "cool" colors.[6] Black, white, and gray, as tones or shades, were shown to be received more positively by males than females.[33]

Historic period [edit]

Children's toys for younger historic period groups are often marketed based on colour, but every bit the age group increases, color becomes less gender-stereotyped.[62] In full general many toys become gender neutral and hence adopt gender-neutral colors. In the Us it is common to associate baby girls with pink and baby boys with blue. This difference in young children is a learned difference rather than an inborn one.[66] Enquiry has looked at the preference of immature children, ages 7 months to 5 years, for minor objects in unlike colors. The results showed that by the age of ii–2.5 years socially constructed gendered colors affects children'south color preference, where girls prefer pink and boys avoid pink, but show no preference for other colors.[66]

Contrary to the adult fondness for blueish, in children xanthous is the nigh favored color, perhaps attributable to its associations with happiness.[38] However, children like colors they find to be pleasant and comforting and their preferences don't modify much, while adult colour preference is usually easily influenced.[six] Slightly older children who have developed a sense of favorite color often tend to selection items that are in that color.[67]

Ecological valence theory has been cited as a possible reason for differences in color preferences between adults and infants.[68] Considering adults have more associations between colors and objects or places from life experiences, their preferences are alter as they get older.

Culture [edit]

Many cultural differences be on perceived colour personality, meaning, and preference. When deciding on brand and product logos, companies should take into account their target consumer, since cultural differences exist. A report looked at colour preference in British and Chinese participants.[64] Each participant was presented with a total of 20 colour swatches one at a time and had to rate the color on 10 unlike emotions. Results showed that British participants and Chinese participants differed on the like-dislike scale the virtually. Chinese participants tended to similar colors that they self rated every bit clean, fresh, and modern, whereas British participants showed no such pattern. When evaluating purchasing intent, color preference affects buying behavior, where liked colors are more than probable to be bought than disliked colors.[fifty] This implies that companies should consider choosing their target consumer beginning so make product colors based on the target's color preferences.

Wollard, (2000)[69] seems to call back that color tin affect one's mood, but the effect also can depend on one'southward civilization and what one's personal reflection may be. For example, someone from Japan may non associate cherry-red with anger, equally people from the U.S. tend to do. Besides, a person who likes the color brown may acquaintance brown with happiness. All the same, Wollard does think that colors can make everyone experience the same, or close to the aforementioned, mood.

Studies have shown people from the aforementioned region, regardless of ethnicity, will have the same color preferences. Common associations connecting colors to a particular emotion may also differ cross-culturally.[6] For instance, i study examined colour relationships with emotion with participants in Germany, Mexico, Poland, Russia, and the United States; finding that red was associated with anger and viewed as strong and active.[lxx] However, only Poles related purple with both acrimony and jealousy while Germans linked jealousy with yellow. This highlights how the influence of dissimilar cultures tin potentially alter perceptions of color and its relationship to emotion.[18]

Sports performance [edit]

Ed Banach wrestles Akira Ohta during the 1984 Summer Olympics.

In item, the colour red has been institute to influence sports performance. During the 2004 Summer Olympics the competitors in boxing, taekwondo, freestyle wrestling, and Greco-Roman wrestling were randomly given blueish or cerise uniforms. A later study constitute that those wearing red won 55% of all the bouts which was a statistically significant increase over the expected fifty%. The colors affected bouts where the competitors were closely matched in ability, where those wearing cherry-red won 60% of the bouts, but non bouts betwixt more unevenly matched competitors. In England, since WWII, teams wearing red uniforms accept averaged college league positions and accept had more than league winners than teams using other colors. In cities with more than ane team, the teams wearing ruddy outperformed the teams wearing other colors. A study of the UEFA Euro 2004 plant similar results. Another study found that those taking penalty kicks performed worst when the goalkeeper had a red uniform. More than anecdotal is the historical dominance of the domestic honors by crimson-wearing teams such AFC Ajax, FC Bayern Munich, Liverpool F.C., and Manchester United F.C. Videos of taekwondo bouts were manipulated in 1 study so that the red and blueish colors of the protective gears were reversed. Both the original and the manipulated videos were shown to referees. The competitors wearing cherry-red were given higher scores despite the videos otherwise being identical. A written report on experienced players of beginning-person shooters establish that those assigned to wear red instead of blue won 55% of the matches.[70]

In that location are several different explanations for this effect. Red is used in finish signs and traffic lights which may associate the color with halting. Red is besides perceived as a strong and active colour which may influence both the person wearing information technology and others. An evolutionary psychology explanation is that cerise may signal health equally opposed to anemic paleness, or indicate anger due to flushing instead of paleness due to fear. It has been argued that detecting flushing may accept influenced the development of primate trichromate vision. Primate studies have institute that some species evaluate rivals and possible mates depending on red color characteristics. Facial redness is associated with testosterone levels in humans, and male skin tends to be redder than female person pare.[70]

Utilise in hospitals [edit]

At the turn of the 20th century, white was widely used in hospitals. In 1914, a surgeon in a San Francisco hospital, Harry Sherman, adopted green, "the complementary colour to hemaglobin" to avoid dazzle. This was adopted by a number of other American hospitals in the following decades. At around the same time, architect William Ludlow began to advocate pale pastel blues and greens in hospitals for therapeutic purposes and advising that "white is negative". In 1930, Dr. Charles Ireland of Guy'due south Hospital in London wrote Colour and Cancer, a book advocating the utilise of concentrated doses of colored light for treating cancer. The practice of using color in hospitals became widespread in the 1930s, particularly promoted by Faber Birren, who established himself as an "industrial color consultant" in 1934 and advised that an environment of soft colors, especially green, would exist soothing for patients.[71]

Gaming [edit]

Since colour is such an important chemical element in how people interpret their surroundings, colour psychology can enhance the feeling of immersion in people that play video games. By using color psychology to crusade immersion in players, players can have less errors playing video games and feel more a part of the game they were playing in comparison to a game that did non have color psychology immersion.[1]

See besides [edit]

  • Color symbolism
  • Color vision
  • Kruithof curve
  • Lüscher color test
  • Visual perception

References [edit]

  1. ^ a b Roohi Due south, Forouzandeh A (May 2019). "Regarding color psychology principles in chance games to heighten the sense of immersion". Entertainment Computing. 30: 100298. doi:ten.1016/j.entcom.2019.100298. ISSN 1875-9521. S2CID 133023544.
  2. ^ a b c de Craen AJ, Roos PJ, de Vries AL, Kleijnen J (1996). "Effect of colour of drugs: systematic review of perceived effect of drugs and of their effectiveness". BMJ. 313 (7072): 1624–1626. doi:10.1136/bmj.313.7072.1624. PMC2359128. PMID 8991013.
  3. ^ a b Modify A (March 21, 2013). "I Run across Reddish". Slate.
  4. ^ a b Birren F (1961). Colour Psychology & Color Therapy. Secaucus, N. J: The Citadel Press. p. 198. ISBN0806506539.
  5. ^ Elliot AJ, Maier MA (2014). "Color psychology: furnishings of perceiving colour on psychological functioning in humans". Annual Review of Psychology. 65: 95–120. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115035. PMID 23808916.
  6. ^ a b c d e f Whitfield TW, Wiltshire TJ (November 1990). "Color psychology: a disquisitional review". Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 116 (iv): 385–411. PMID 2289687.
  7. ^ "Why You Experience Hungry When You See The Colour Crimson". So Yummy. 2019-01-14. Retrieved 2021-09-08 .
  8. ^ Findlay P (2018-09-12). "The ketchup and mustard theory of advertising, AKA learning to taste your make". Mumbrella . Retrieved 2021-09-08 .
  9. ^ "Colour History". Colour Therapy Healing. 2016-04-01. Retrieved 2021-11-20 .
  10. ^ Riley, Charles A. II. "Color Codes: Mod Theories of Color in Philosophy, Painting and Architecture, Literature, Music, and Psychology". Hanover: University Press of New England, 1995, p. 307.
  11. ^ Elliot AJ, Maier MA (2014-01-03). "Color psychology: furnishings of perceiving colour on psychological functioning in humans". Annual Review of Psychology. 65 (one): 95–120. doi:x.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115035. PMID 23808916.
  12. ^ a b Shankar MU, Levitan C, Prescott J, Spence C (2009-04-28). "The Influence of Color and Label Information on Flavor Perception". Chemosensory Perception. 2 (two): 53–58. doi:10.1007/s12078-009-9046-4. ISSN 1936-5802. S2CID 59026706.
  13. ^ Bleicher Southward (2012). Contemporary Colour: Theory & Employ. New York: Delmar. pp. 48, 50. ISBN978-ane-1335-7997-7.
  14. ^ Shankar MU, Levitan CA, Spence C (March 2010). "Grape expectations: the office of cognitive influences in color-flavor interactions". Consciousness and Cognition. 19 (1): 380–390. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.008. PMID 19828330. S2CID 32230245.
  15. ^ Masahiro Shibasaki, Nobuo Masataka (2014) "The color carmine distorts fourth dimension perception for men, but non for women" Scientific Reports 4, Article number: 5899 doi:ten.1038/srep05899
  16. ^ Beau Lotto in "Do You Encounter What I Run into" (2011)
  17. ^ Makwana Chiliad (2018-02-26). "The effects of color on time perception – Blue stimuli are temporally overestimated". Timing Research Forum . Retrieved 2021-ten-02 .
  18. ^ a b c d eastward Shevell SK, Kingdom FA (2008). "Color in circuitous scenes". Almanac Review of Psychology. 59: 143–166. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093619. PMID 18154500. S2CID 24460261.
  19. ^ Baraniuk C. "Can blue lights prevent suicide at railroad train stations?". www.bbc.com . Retrieved 2020-03-xi .
  20. ^ "Blue streetlights believed to prevent suicides, street crime". The Seattle Times. 2008-12-11. Archived from the original on September thirteen, 2010.
  21. ^ Shimbun Y (Dec 10, 2008). "Blueish streetlights may forestall criminal offense, suicide". Archived from the original on 2009-10-09. Retrieved 2010-02-18 .
  22. ^ "Can Blue-Colored Light Forestall Suicide?". Archived from the original on 2017-12-15. Retrieved 2010-02-18 .
  23. ^ Volition Blueish Lights Reduce Suicides in Nihon?
  24. ^ Dolinska B (1999). "Empirical investigation into placebo effectiveness" (PDF). Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine. 16 (2): 57–58. doi:x.1017/s0790966700005176. Archived from the original (w) on 2011-07-22. Retrieved 2009-04-29 .
  25. ^ Lamancusa K. "Emotional Reactions to Color". Artistic Latitude. Archived from the original on 2016-03-11. Retrieved 2016-03-thirty .
  26. ^ Dutton, Denis. 2003. 'Aesthetics and Evolutionary Psychology' in "The Oxford Handbook for Aesthetics". Oxford Academy Press.
  27. ^ a b c d "Color Assignment - Past Joe Hallock". world wide web.joehallock.com . Retrieved 2020-03-26 .
  28. ^ Palmer SE, Schloss KB (May 2010). "An ecological valence theory of homo color preference". Proceedings of the National University of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (xix): 8877–8882. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.8877P. doi:x.1073/pnas.0906172107. PMC2889342. PMID 20421475.
  29. ^ Lichtlé MC (2007-01-01). "The effect of an advertisement's colour on emotions evoked by attitude towards the ad". International Periodical of Advertising. 26 (1): 37–62. doi:10.1080/02650487.2007.11072995. ISSN 0265-0487. S2CID 142662891.
  30. ^ Labrecque LI, Milne GR (2012). "Exciting Red and Competent Blue: The Importance of Color in Marketing". Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. xl (5): 711–727. doi:x.1007/s11747-010-0245-y. S2CID 167731928.
  31. ^ Connecting With Color
  32. ^ Fernández-Vázquez R (2011). "Visual and Instrumental Evaluation of Orange Juice Color: A Consumers' Preference Study". Journal of Sensory Studies. 26 (half dozen): 436–444. doi:x.1111/j.1745-459X.2011.00360.x.
  33. ^ a b c d eastward Singh S (2006). "Impact of color on marketing". Direction Decision. 44 (6): 783–789. doi:10.1108/00251740610673332.
  34. ^ a b c d Bottomley PA, Doyle JR (2006). "The interactive effects of colors and products on perceptions of make logo appropriateness". Marketing Theory. 6 (i): 63–83. doi:10.1177/1470593106061263. S2CID 53464180.
  35. ^ O'Connor Z. "Logo colour and differentiation: A new awarding of colour mapping". Color Research & Awarding, 36 (1), p55-60 . Retrieved 2016-03-30 .
  36. ^ a b c d eastward f g h i j k fifty m Labrecque LI, Milne GR (2011). "Exciting red and competent bluish: the importance of color in marketing". Periodical of the Academy of Marketing Science. twoscore (5): 711–727. doi:10.1007/s11747-010-0245-y. S2CID 167731928.
  37. ^ Warner 50, Franzen R (June 1947). "Value of color in advertising". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 31 (3): 260–270. doi:x.1037/h0057772. PMID 20241978.
  38. ^ a b c d due east f chiliad h i j k l m n o p q Aslam MM (2006). "Are You Selling the Correct Color? A Cross-cultural Review of Color every bit a Marketing Cue". Journal of Marketing Communications. 12 (i): 15–30. doi:10.1080/13527260500247827. S2CID 168153362.
  39. ^ a b Elliot AJ (2015-04-02). "Colour and psychological functioning: a review of theoretical and empirical work". Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 368. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00368. PMC4383146. PMID 25883578.
  40. ^ Piotrowski C, Armstrong T (2012). "Colour Red: Implications for applied psychology and marketing research". Psychology and Pedagogy. 49 (i–2): 55–57.
  41. ^ a b Lupton E (2017). Pattern is storytelling. New York, NY. ISBN978-1-942303-19-0. OCLC 982650081.
  42. ^ Edward T. Vieira, Jr., Public Relations Planning: A Strategic Approach (2018), p. 230.
  43. ^ Kauppinen-Räisänen H, Jauffret MN (2018-01-08). "Using colour semiotics to explore colour meanings". Qualitative Market Research. 21 (1): 101–117. doi:10.1108/QMR-03-2016-0033. ISSN 1352-2752.
  44. ^ Tyner, Judith A., Principles of Map Design, New York: Guilford Press, 2010, p.64
  45. ^ Schloss KB, Palmer SE (February 2011). "Aesthetic response to color combinations: preference, harmony, and similarity". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 73 (2): 551–571. doi:ten.3758/s13414-010-0027-0. PMC3037488. PMID 21264737.
  46. ^ Deng X, Hui SK, Huntchinson J (2010). "Consumer preferences for color combinations: An empirical analysis of similarity-based colour". Journal of Consumer Psychology. twenty (4): 476–484. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2010.07.005.
  47. ^ a b c d Skorinko JL, Kemmer S, Hebl MR, Lane DM (2006). "fifteen. A Rose by Any Other Name...: Color-Naming Influences on Decision Making". Psychology & Marketing. 23 (12): 975–993. CiteSeerXx.1.i.581.1374. doi:x.1002/mar.20142.
  48. ^ a b c Babin BJ, Hardesty DM, Suter TA (2003). "Colour and shopping intentions". Journal of Business Research. 56 (vii): 541–551. doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(01)00246-6.
  49. ^ a b Miller EG, Kahn BE (2005). "Shades of Meaning: The Effect of Colour and Season Names on Consumer Option". Journal of Consumer Research. 32 (i): 86–92. CiteSeerX10.1.1.488.3177. doi:x.1086/429602.
  50. ^ a b c d Kauppinen-Raisanen H, Luomala HT (2010). "Exploring consumers' production-specific colour meanings". Qualitative Market Inquiry. 13 (3): 287–308. doi:10.1108/13522751011053644.
  51. ^ a b Kawasaki M, Yamaguchi Y (January 2012). "Effects of subjective preference of colors on attending-related occipital theta oscillations". NeuroImage. 59 (one): 808–814. doi:x.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.042. PMID 21820064.
  52. ^ Cugelman, B. Cugeman, R. et al. (2019). Colour Psychology. AlterSpark. https://www.alterspark.com/color-psychology
  53. ^ a b Bellizzi JA, Crowley AE, Hasty RW (1983). "The effects of color in store design". Periodical of Retailing. 59 (1): 21–45.
  54. ^ Mao T, Yang J, Ru T, Chen Q, Shi H, Zhou J, Zhou One thousand (2018). "Does cherry light induce people to be riskier? Exploring the colored light effect on the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART)". Periodical of Ecology Psychology. 57: 73–82. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.07.001. S2CID 149927011.
  55. ^ a b c Withrow RL (2004). "The Use of Color in Art Therapy". The Journal of Humanistic Counseling, Educational activity and Development. 43 (1): 33–40. doi:10.1002/j.2164-490X.2004.tb00040.x. ISSN 2164-490X.
  56. ^ Azeemi ST, Rafiq HM, Ismail I, Kazmi SR, Azeemi A (October 2019). "The mechanistic basis of chromotherapy: Current knowledge and future perspectives". Complementary Therapies in Medicine. 46: 217–222. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2019.08.025. PMID 31519282. S2CID 202571445.
  57. ^ Samina T. Yousuf Azeemi, Mohsin Raza, "A Critical Analysis of Chromotherapy and Its Scientific Development", Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2,Article ID 254639, viii pages, 2005. https://doi.org/10.1093/ecam/neh137
  58. ^ Paragas ED, Ng AT, Reyes DV, Reyes GA (2019-05-01). "Effects of Chromotherapy on the Cognitive Ability of Older Adults: A Quasi-Experimental Study". Explore. 15 (three): 191–197. doi:10.1016/j.explore.2019.01.002. PMID 30718190. S2CID 73443325.
  59. ^ Glickman 1000, Byrne B, Pineda C, Hauck WW, Brainard GC (March 2006). "Light therapy for seasonal affective disorder with blue narrow-band light-emitting diodes (LEDs)". Biological Psychiatry. 59 (six): 502–507. doi:x.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.006. PMID 16165105. S2CID 42586876.
  60. ^ Swan J (2003). Jonathan Swan'southward quack magic: the dubious history of wellness fads and cures. London: Ebury Press. ISBN978-0-09-188809-1. OCLC 52515553.
  61. ^ Tofle, R.B. (2004). "Color in Healthcare Environments - A Research Study" (PDF). California: Coalition for Health Environments Research. Retrieved November xix, 2021.
  62. ^ a b c Hull JH, Hull DB, Knopp C (2011). "The Affect of color on ratings of 'daughter' and 'boy' toys". North American Journal of Psychology. 13 (3): 549–562. 276353296. Archived from the original on 2016-10-06. Retrieved 2017-02-fourteen .
  63. ^ a b Auster CJ, Mansbach CS (2012). "The Gender Marketing of Toys: An Analysis of Color and Blazon of Toy on the Disney Store Website". Sexual practice Roles. 67 (7–viii): 375–388. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0177-8. S2CID 143551702.
  64. ^ a b Ou LC, Luo MR, Woodcock A, Wright A (2004). "A written report of colour emotion and colour preference. Part I: Colour emotions for single colours". Color Research & Application. 29 (three): 232–240. doi:10.1002/col.20010.
  65. ^ Ou LC, Luo MR, Woodcock A, Wright A (2004). "A report of colour emotion and colour preference. Part Two: Color emotions for two-color combinations". Color Research & Application. 29 (4): 292–298. doi:x.1002/col.20024.
  66. ^ a b Lobue V, Deloache JS (September 2011). "Pretty in pinkish: The early evolution of gender-stereotyped colour preferences". The British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 29 (Pt 3): 656–667. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02027.x. PMID 21848751. S2CID 23219132.
  67. ^ Gollety M, Guichard N (2011). "The dilemma of season and color in the choice of packaging by children". Immature Consumers. 12 (i): 82–xc. doi:10.1108/17473611111114803.
  68. ^ Taylor C, Schloss K, Palmer SE, Franklin A (October 2013). "Color preferences in infants and adults are dissimilar". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 20 (5): 916–922. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0411-six. PMID 23435629. S2CID 10261601.
  69. ^ "Wollard, K. (2000). Orange you glad y'all're not blueish?". [ permanent expressionless link ]
  70. ^ a b c Diana Widermann, Robert A. Barton, and Russel A. Hill. Evolutionary perspectives on sport and competition. In Roberts SC (2011). Roberts SC (ed.). Practical Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford Academy Printing. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN9780199586073.
  71. ^ Pantalony D (September 2009). "The color of medicine". CMAJ. 181 (6–7): 402–403. doi:10.1503/cmaj.091058. PMC2742127. PMID 19737828.

pettybadmight.blogspot.com

Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Color_psychology

0 Response to "Apple Is Happy to Use Women and People of Color as Art Not Authority"

Postar um comentário

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel